Case Studies of Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction in Elderly Patients

A B S T R A C T

Background and Objective: Postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) involves decline in several cognitive domains after surgery and is particularly common after cardiac surgery. Given the potential effects of such cognitive dysfunction on quality of life, it is important to study it in multiple populations in order to limit its occurrence. Recent advances in surgical technology may assist in achieving this goal.
Methods: We present the long-term neuropsychological outcome of two elderly patients, one of whom had off pump heart surgery and the other oncological surgery. We administered a series of neuropsychological tests assessing attention, complex scanning, verbal working memory, executive functioning, short-term and long-term memory, and visuospatial perception before surgery, prior to discharge, at 3-month follow-up and 6 years after surgery. We compared the performance of these two patients to normative datasets.
Results: Despite equivalent levels of pre-surgery performance between the two patients, the oncology patient exceeded his preoperative neurocognitive levels, suggesting less postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the heart patient overall, on all neuropsychological domains at 6-year follow-up, except short-term retention. In contrast, the heart patient showed no improvement, and, instead, showed some cognitive decline which remained consistent over time.
Conclusion: Our findings highlight the critical role of the type of surgery utilized in the development of POCD and have implications for clinical management and patients’ quality of life in the very long term.

Keywords

Postoperative cognitive dysfunction, neuropsychological tests, surgery, heart surgery, breast cancer, cognitive domains, quality of life

Key Points

• Postoperative cognitive dysfunction assessing cognitive domains with neuropsychological tests is necessary in heart surgery and breast cancer for optimal quality of life.
• The research highlights the critical role of the type of surgery utilized in the development of POCD and have implications for clinical management and patients’ quality of life in the very long term.

Introduction

Postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) refers to impairment in one or more cognitive domains after surgery. This may include decrements in attention, orientation, memory, and learning and may improve to baseline levels by three months after surgery [1, 2]. The severity of POCD covers a broad range, varying from mild cognitive decline to severe dementia and postoperative delirium [2]. Researchers suggest that approximately a quarter of elderly patients who undergo major surgery suffer from cognition dysfunction and 50% of these patients will experience a permanent dysfunction [3]. While present after various types of surgery, it is most common after cardiac surgery employing cardiopulmonary bypass [4]. The reason of cognitive decline after anaesthesia and surgery remains unclear, but research indicates that this could be a result from the anaesthesia, the surgery, the patient, or their combination. The temporal relationship to anaesthesia and surgery that identified, does not imply causation. Although poor cerebral oxygenation can lead to poor cerebral and cognitive outcomes, cognitive decline after surgery in the elderly can be developed in the absence of cerebral hypoxemia [5].

Heart surgery often leads to cognitive impairment, in spite of life-saving potential. Extracorporeal circulation (ECC) often causes side effects varying from mild to severe vascular cerebral stroke, as well as renal and pulmonary complications, and systemic inflammatory response [2]. The extensive influence of ECC, not surprisingly, may have an adverse impact on cognitive functions. A comprehensive neuropsychological assessment is necessary for the detection and specification of POCD, as it is sometimes quite subtle and may elude detection [6]. The incidence of postoperative cognitive decline among cardiac surgery patients is far from trivial. Specifically, studies have reported rates ranging from 11% to 80%, and it persists in as many as 42% of patients up to 3-5 years later, referred to as long-term POCD [7, 8]. In contrast, the incidence of POCD after non-cardiac surgery appears to be much lower, showing substantial improvement over time; the prevalence rate one week after surgery is 25.8%, at 3 months it is 9.9 % and at 2 years it is 1% [4].

Cancer survivors are exposed to several risk factors for cognitive dysfunction, such as general anaesthesia, surgical trauma, among others. Sekiguchi et al. (2016) examined patients with breast cancer who underwent surgery, 1 week and 6 months after surgery and showed attentional dysfunction and thalamic volume reduction immediately after surgery [9, 10]. This is considered due to changes in brain structure, particularly in the thalamus, that occurs immediately after surgery. Patients would develop clinical POCD are those with risk factors of POCD, such as the severity of surgery, the occurrence of complications, and pre-existing cognitive impairments [10]. PCCI caused by chemotherapeutic drugs that are cytotoxic affecting both normal and cancer cells, they contribute to cognitive impairment following chemotherapy treatment. Cognitive dysfunction is common following other anticancer treatments such as radiation therapy, hormonal therapy, surgery, or in patients with noncentral nervous system cancer, and has important effects on brain functioning.

In addition, several studies suggest that clinical manifestations of cognitive dysfunction may occur in cancer patients, prior to chemotherapy or in patients who are not treated with cancer therapies. Because of the above, some researchers suggest that it should be more correct if we use the term cancer-related cognitive dysfunction [11]. In addition, Megari (2020) have found that breast cancer patients who received chemotherapy, performed worse compared to patients with prostate, colorectal and thyroid cancer in cognitive tasks including attention, memory, executive functions and language [12]. Moreover, breast cancer patients had significantly greater difficulty in performing IADL compared to patients with other types of cancer [13]. Several investigators have speculated regarding the possible mechanisms of POCD suggesting intraoperative cerebral ischaemia and cerebral oxygen desaturation, caused by hypoperfusion, arrhythmias, rapid rewarming and inflammation, either local or global, as potential risk factors [14, 15].

As regards the etiologic role of cardiopulmonary bypass and anaesthesia in the development of POCD, the main fact is that it remains quite controversial [2]. Among potential contributory factors that have not been well established, is the subtle blood pressure change during coronary artery bypass surgery that can cause watershed strokes, as well as the drugs associated with anaesthesia [16, 17]. In addition, it has been hypothesized that nonspecific effects of surgery, such as postoperative pain, medications, and sleep disturbance, may also be contributing factors for POCD [18]. Demographic and clinical factors that are correlated with postoperative cognitive dysfunction include the patient’s age, educational level, preoperative cognitive performance and comorbidity [19]. Older patients, with limited education and comorbid cerebrovascular disease are the most likely to show POCD [2, 20]. Thus, the pathogenesis of postoperative cognitive decline remains unclear, as it may be a multifactorial phenomenon.

POCD may have additional sequelae. It has been found to be associated with increased morbidity and mortality, prolonged hospitalization, and increased health care costs, and may have an adverse impact on social functioning and health-related quality of life [21]. Most critically, it has important social implications for patients as well as their caregivers, as it can limit patients’ social functioning, independent living and autonomy [2]. Therefore, it would be important to assess all patients post-surgically for this condition. Perhaps due to the multifactorial nature of the pathogenesis of POCD, there does not appear to be a single intervention that might provide adequate protection of the brain during surgery [14]. Prevention is the best treatment for many diseases and in case of POCD, early recognition and management of potential perioperative risk factors can lead to optimal outcome of patients who undergo surgical procedures. Long-term follow-up of POCD is essential in understanding the factors related to its development and treatment. Most long-term follow-up assessments have been conducted as early as one month and as late as five years post-surgery [22]. In the present study, we investigated two patients at four time points: at baseline (pre-surgery), before discharge (7-10 days post-surgery), three months post-surgery and 6 years post-surgery.

Table 1: Patients’ demographic and clinical characteristics.

Characteristic

Case 1

Case 2

Type of surgery

Heart

Oncology

Age (yrs)

76

75

Education (yrs)

12

10

Gender

Male

Female

Surgery time (min)

88

93

Length of ICU stay (days)

2

1


Methods

I Participants

Participants in this study were two patients. One suffering from CAD scheduled for elective off-pump coronary artery bypass grafting and the other patient with breast cancer scheduled for mastectomy. The two study patients were matched regarding their demographic and clinical characteristics, as seen in (Table 1). Exclusion criteria for the original neuropsychological study were a pre-existing psychiatric or neurological disorder, inability to cooperate with a neuropsychological assessment, as judged by the experimenter, carotid artery stenosis greater than 60%, as assessed with duplex ultrasonography, a history of substance and alcohol abuse, and cancer patients receiving chemotherapy or radiation therapy. A description of the two patients follows:

i Case Report I

B.C is a 76-year-old man with 12 years of education, who had worked as an administrator in an office. He is married and has two adult children. Other than his CAD, he had a negative medical history. He underwent off pump cardiac surgery and received neuropsychological testing before surgery, prior to discharge (7-10 days after surgery), at three-months postoperatively, and 6 years after surgery.

ii Case Report II

P.H. is a 75-year-old woman with 10 years of education, who had worked as a nurse. She is married and has three adult children. She, too, had a negative medical history other than her cancer problem, and underwent surgical mastectomy. A neuropsychological assessment was conducted before surgery, prior to discharge (7-10 days after surgery), at three-month follow-up, as well as 6 years postoperatively.

II Procedure

We administered a battery of neuropsychological tests and assessed all major cognitive domains (visual-spatial perception, executive functions, complex scanning and visual tracking, attention, verbal working memory, and short- and long-term memory). The battery included tests with multiple versions or tests that could be adapted to multiple administrations in order to avoid potential practice effects in follow-up testing. Also included were scales measuring anxiety, depression, and positive and negative mood, as neuropsychological performance may be influenced by a patient’s mood state (Table 2). This assessment required approximately 60 minutes for each patient to complete and was blind to patient management. We investigated cognitive changes prior to discharge, three months and six years after surgery, in each patient by comparing their follow up scores to pre-surgery baseline scores on each test as well as normative data. The criterion of change that we chose to apply was that of 20% decrease. We defined cognitive dysfunction as a postoperative 20% decrease, relative to pre-surgery performance, on 20% of neuropsychological tests at each follow up time point [7].

Table 2: Raw scores of neuropsychological performance and mood ratings of two patients at four time points.

Test

Time-point

Case 1

Case 2

 

Stroop

Word

T1

57

60

T2

 36

59

T3

 38

69

T4

 38

69

 

Color

T1

45

49

T2

38

47

T3

40

55

 

T4

38

54

 

Color-Word

T1

25

26

T2

 12

23

T3

23

28

 

T4

21

38

Fuld Object Memory Test

Storage

T1

42

40

T2

 31

44

T3

 32

46

 

T4

35

47

 

Retrieval

T1

30

32

T2

31

33

T3

29

36

 

T4

31

36

 

Repeated retrieval

T1

23

23

T2

17

23

T3

18

27

 

T4

17

27

 

Long-term recall

T1

  8

  8

T2

  6

  7

T3

  7

10

 

T4

  7

10

 

Long-term recognition

T1

  2

  2

T2

  4

  3

T3

  3

  0

 

T4

  3

  0

 

Ineffective reminders

T1

  5

  4

T2

  5

  2

T3

  6

  1

 

T4

  6

  0

 

Symbol Digit Modalities Test

T1

51

55

T2

38

51

T3

41

57

 

T4

41

57

 

Judgment of Line Orientation

T1

  7

  7

T2

  5

  7

T3

  6

  8

 

T4

  6

  8

 

Digit Span

Forward

T1

  6

  6

T2

  4

  6

T3

  4

10

 

T4

  4

10

 

Backward

T1

 4

  4

T2

   2

  4

T3

  3

  5

 

T4

  3

  5

Scale

 

 

 

 

PANAS

Positive Affect

T1

40

41

T2

37

41

T3

38

41

 

T4

35

34

 

Negative Affect

 

T1

16

19

T2

16

16

T3

15

13

 

T4

18

12

 

Geriatric Depression Scale

T1

  1

  1

T2

  3

  0

T3

  2

  0

 

T4

  4

  2

 

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

T1

35

37

T2

32

31

T3

30

28

 

T4

28

22

‡ This mark indicates cognitive decline relative to normative data and according to the 20% decrease criterion.


Results

The two patients did not differ from each other, nor from the normative datasets, on their neuropsychological performance preoperatively. Yet post-surgery, the two patients’ cognitive performance diverged (Table 2). Specifically, according to the 1 SD criterion, Case 1 had cognitive decline on attention at the 3-month follow-up time point, on executive functions immediately postoperatively and on short-term memory at two follow up time points. In contrast, Case 2 had increased performance at three-month and at six-year follow-up after surgery, compared to his preoperative performance, with no cognitive decline. With respect to mood measures, neither patient had elevated scores on depression, anxiety, or positive and negative mood scales, nor did they show significant change over time.

Discussion

Our findings indicated different patterns of change postoperatively, despite equivalent levels of cognitive abilities preoperatively. Specifically, the cancer patient had no cognitive decline postoperatively, and, in fact, showed increased performance on attention, executive functioning, short-term and long-term memory, verbal working memory, complex scanning and visual-spatial perception across the four time points. In contrast, the heart patient showed some cognitive decline, specifically on attention, executive functions, verbal working memory and short-term memory, which remained consistent over time, and never reached his pre-surgery baseline neurocognitive levels. Both cases showed consistency at six years after surgery, relative to the three-month follow-up, indicating that their cognitive status remained stable over time, and that any spontaneous recovery had ceased by three months postoperatively. In conclusion, the present findings suggest long term improvement of cancer patient and failure to reach preoperative levels after heart surgery. This improvement of cognitive functioning among cancer patients has implications for resuming social and occupational roles, as well as for quality of life after surgery. In contrast, heart operations are associated with an increased risk of cognitive decline three months after surgery that appears not to improve over the course of a six-year follow-up, with implications for reduced daily functioning.

Some limitations of the present study should be considered, with respect to the generalizability of our findings. Long term outcome was available only for two patients. Future investigations should include more comparison groups, such as non-surgical patients with coronary artery disease who are treated medically, or patients undergoing other major non-cardiac surgery with general anaesthesia. These comparison groups could help to clarify the respective roles of surgical procedures in developing POCD. Future research is necessary to study the role of variables such as age and degree of pre-existing cerebrovascular disease, peripheral vascular disease, or diabetes and personality factors such as Type D personality -- the so-called distressed personality (with negative mood state and social inhibition) -- as predictors of cognitive outcomes after CABG. Thus, the present findings suggest the influence of the type of surgery (heart or oncology) on cognitive functioning and a partial etiologic role of cardiopulmonary bypass on POCD. The clinical implications are substantial as POCD can have an adverse impact on quality of life and social functioning [23]. In addition, it is a multifactorial problem that may have a serious social and economic impact on society. Any surgery-related technique or procedure with the potential of reducing POCD following surgery may be of great benefit to the patient and his/her family.

Funding

None.

Article Info

Article Type
Case Report
Publication history
Received: Mon 02, Aug 2021
Accepted: Wed 10, Nov 2021
Published: Fri 26, Nov 2021
Copyright
© 2023 Kalliopi Megari. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Hosting by Science Repository.
DOI: 10.31487/j.IJCST.2021.03.02

Author Info

Corresponding Author
Kalliopi Megari
Lab of Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Thessaloniki, Greece

Figures & Tables

Table 1: Patients’ demographic and clinical characteristics.

Characteristic

Case 1

Case 2

Type of surgery

Heart

Oncology

Age (yrs)

76

75

Education (yrs)

12

10

Gender

Male

Female

Surgery time (min)

88

93

Length of ICU stay (days)

2

1


Table 2: Raw scores of neuropsychological performance and mood ratings of two patients at four time points.

Test

Time-point

Case 1

Case 2

 

Stroop

Word

T1

57

60

T2

 36

59

T3

 38

69

T4

 38

69

 

Color

T1

45

49

T2

38

47

T3

40

55

 

T4

38

54

 

Color-Word

T1

25

26

T2

 12

23

T3

23

28

 

T4

21

38

Fuld Object Memory Test

Storage

T1

42

40

T2

 31

44

T3

 32

46

 

T4

35

47

 

Retrieval

T1

30

32

T2

31

33

T3

29

36

 

T4

31

36

 

Repeated retrieval

T1

23

23

T2

17

23

T3

18

27

 

T4

17

27

 

Long-term recall

T1

  8

  8

T2

  6

  7

T3

  7

10

 

T4

  7

10

 

Long-term recognition

T1

  2

  2

T2

  4

  3

T3

  3

  0

 

T4

  3

  0

 

Ineffective reminders

T1

  5

  4

T2

  5

  2

T3

  6

  1

 

T4

  6

  0

 

Symbol Digit Modalities Test

T1

51

55

T2

38

51

T3

41

57

 

T4

41

57

 

Judgment of Line Orientation

T1

  7

  7

T2

  5

  7

T3

  6

  8

 

T4

  6

  8

 

Digit Span

Forward

T1

  6

  6

T2

  4

  6

T3

  4

10

 

T4

  4

10

 

Backward

T1

 4

  4

T2

   2

  4

T3

  3

  5

 

T4

  3

  5

Scale

 

 

 

 

PANAS

Positive Affect

T1

40

41

T2

37

41

T3

38

41

 

T4

35

34

 

Negative Affect

 

T1

16

19

T2

16

16

T3

15

13

 

T4

18

12

 

Geriatric Depression Scale

T1

  1

  1

T2

  3

  0

T3

  2

  0

 

T4

  4

  2

 

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

T1

35

37

T2

32

31

T3

30

28

 

T4

28

22

‡ This mark indicates cognitive decline relative to normative data and according to the 20% decrease criterion.


References

1.     Rasmussen LS (2006) Postoperative cognitive dysfunction: incidence and prevention. Best Pract Res Clin Anaesthesiol 20: 315-330. [Crossref]

2.     Selnes OA, Gottesman RF (2010) Neuropsychological outcomes after coronary artery bypass grafting. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 16: 221-226. [Crossref]

3.     Kotekar N, Shenkar A, Nagaraj R (2018) Postoperative cognitive dysfunction - current preventive strategies. Clin Interv Aging 13: 2267-2273. [Crossref]

4.     Mackensen GB, Gelb AW (2004) Postoperative cognitive deficits: more questions than answers. Eur J Anaesthesiol 21: 85-88. [Crossref]

5.     Evered LA, Silbert BS (2018) Postoperative Cognitive Dysfunction and Noncardiac Surgery. Anesth Analg 127: 496-505. [Crossref]

6.     Megari K (2019) The contribution of neuropsychology in cardiac surgery: Cognitive deficits after heart surgery can be detected only by neuropsychological assessment. Ciru Cardi En Méxi 4: 51-53.

7.     Slater JP, Guarino T, Stack J, Vinod K, Bustami RT et al. (2009) Cerebral oxygen desaturation predicts cognitive decline and longer hospital stay after cardiac surgery. Ann Thorac Surg 87: 36-45. [Crossref]

8.     Argiriadou H, Megari K, Antonitsis P, Kosmidis MH, Papakonstantinou C et al. (2010) Non-pulsatile circulation with axial-flow left ventricular assist device preserves neurocognitive function. Perfusion 25: 225-228. [Crossref]

9.     Sekiguchi A, Sato C, Matsudaira I, Kotozaki Y, Nouchi R et al. (2016) Postoperative hormonal therapy prevents recovery of neurological damage after surgery in patients with breast cancer. Sci Rep 6: 34671. [Crossref]

10.  Sato C, Sekiguchi A, Kawai M, Kotozaki Y, Nouchi R et al. (2015) Postoperative Structural Brain Changes and Cognitive Dysfunction in Patients with Breast Cancer. PLoS One 10: e0140655. [Crossref]

11.  Megari K (2021) Neuropsychological functioning among patients with different types of cancer: Postchemotherapy cognitive impairment and implications for rehabilitation. Neuropsychiatr 35: 92-97. [Crossref]

12.  Megari K (2020) Language impairment among patients with different types of cancer. Comparison with healthy adults. Breast Cancer Res Treat 184: 779-782. [Crossref]

13.  Megari K (2021) Instrumental activities of daily living and neuropsychological functioning among patients with different types of cancer. Eur J Neurol 28: 66-70. [Crossref]

14.  Patel N, Minhas JS, Chung EML (2015) Risk Factors Associated with Cognitive Decline after Cardiac Surgery: A Systematic Review. Cardiovasc Psychiatry Neurol 15: 1-13. [Crossref]

15.  Pappa M, Theodosiadis N, Tsounis A, Sarafis P (2017) Pathogenesis and treatment of post-operative cognitive dysfunction. Electron Physician 9: 3768-3775. [Crossref]

16.  Gottesman RF, Sherman PM, Grega MA, Yousem DM, Borowicz LM et al. (2006) Watershed strokes after cardiac surgery: diagnosis, etiology and outcome. Stroke 37: 2306-2311. [Crossref]

17.  Hanning CD (2005) Postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Br J Anaesth 95: 82-87. [Crossref]

18.  Selnes OA, Gottesman RF, Grega MA, Baumgartner WA, Zeger SL et al. (2012) Cognitive and neurologic outcomes after coronary-artery bypass surgery. N Engl J Med 366: 250-257. [Crossref]

19.  Kattan MA, Alnimari AE, Siraj AF, Humied AA, Alsuqati FA (2018) Postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) and neuroinflammation associated with cardiac surgery: a review. Int J Med Dev Coun 3: 136-140.

20.  Megari K, Kosmidis MH, Anastasiadis K (2011) Postoperative cognitive decline in the elderly following cardiac surgery. Paper presented at the 5th International Congress on Brain & Behaviour of the International Society on Brain & Behaviour & 18th Thessaloniki Conference of the South-East European Society for Neurology and Psychiatry, 3-5 November, Thessaloniki, Greece.

21.  Megari K (2013) Quality of Life in Chronic Disease Patients. Health Psychol Res 1: e27. [Crossref]

22.  Symes E, Maruff P, Ajani A, Currie J (2000) Issues associated with the identification of cognitive change following coronary artery bypass grafting. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 34: 770-784. [Crossref]

23. Anastasiadis K, Argiriadou H, Kosmidis MΗ, Megari K, Antonitsis P et al. (2011) Neurocognitive outcome after coronary artery bypass surgery using minimal versus conventional extracorporeal circulation: a randomized controlled pilot study. Heart 97: 1082-1088. [Crossref]